INTRODUCTION TO VOLUME 1:

THEORY AND METHOD

 

 

Jonathan Potter

Professor of Discourse Analysis

 

 

Discourse and Rhetoric Group                                          Email:     J.A.Potter@lboro.ac.uk

Department of Social Sciences                                        Tel:          01509 223384

Loughborough University                                                      Fax:         01509 223944

Loughborough                                                                                                                

Leicestershire, LE11 3TU               

 

 


These three volumes will select from a tradition of research that starts with psychology as something that is understood, expressed and described in discourse.   That is, it is research that considers psychology as a practical object for people while interacting with one another, either directly or through texts or other written forms.   It covers, for example, research on how conversation can display a psychological state, and how such a display can be treated as upset, say, or manipulative..  It surveys work on the way attitudes or dispositions are invoked in arguments.  It describes studies that look at the way identity is related to actions, to criticism and linked into to broader categories such as ethnic or national groups. 

What collects this tradition of work together is its focus on psychology as something practical for individuals living their lives in families, workplaces or in specialized environments such as psychodynamic therapy, medical consultations or child protection helplines.  Indeed, it starts with life as something practical and lived with others; that is, as something relational.  Memories, identities, perceptions and all the other technical objects of traditional psychology are studied for the way they enter peoples’ practices, they treated as something inseparable from the practices in which they come alive.  The interest is in the sense they have for the texture of peoples’ lives.  They are things that are invoked, constructed, managed, and denied as people flirt, and argue, or as they build up damning cases in courtrooms or as they develop criticisms of social groups in political speeches.  Indeed, some of these technical objects need to be understood very differently than in traditional psychological work and some disappear altogether.  This tradition thus ends up with a very different picture of psychology than is common in the mainstream (social) cognitive tradition.

In this introduction to the Volume 1 I will perform a number of tasks.  First, I will give some further account of the content of the volumes.  This will involve exploring some of the traditions of work that are not represented and explaining their absence.  It will also involve trying to make explicit the rationale for the specific articles that I have selected out of a much broader and deeper pool of excellent possibilities.  Second, I will discuss the origins of this tradition of work and highlight the different intellectual threads that it weaves together.  This will provide a frame for reading some of the contributions and will therefore be the major part of the introduction.  Third, and finally, I will discuss the methodological work that comprises the last section of Volume 1.  I will try and show its development and highlight some of the contemporary issues that it raises.

 

The scope of Discourse and Psychology

 

The tradition of work represented here includes discursive psychology, some areas of discourse analysis, some work inspired by linguistic philosophy and constructionism, some work inspired by poststructural thinking, and some ethnomethodology and conversation analysis.  Before offering a more precise characterization it is perhaps more straightforward to explain why some traditions of work are not represented here.  As an interdisciplinary field lying not just between psychology and discourse studies, but bounded by linguistics, sociology, anthropology and communication there is ample potential for confusion.

First, note that these are volumes on discourse and psychology.  They are not collections of work on language and psychology; nor are they focused on linguistics and psychology.  The term discourse picks out the focus on language-in-use (conversation analysts write about talk-in-interaction) rather than language-in-the-abstract as a set of words, word meanings and rules for combination.  Discourse is talk and text embedded in social practices of one kind or another (complaining about noisy neighbours, arguing about the royal family, offering therapeutic advice).  The topic here is the activity and its role within the setting.  It is also the way the discourse contributes to the structure of the setting itself.  Ethnomethodologists have it that talk is both context sensitive and context reproducing (Heritage, 1984).  The analysis that is offered in chapters below is not linguistic analysis.  It starts with actions in settings and although the focus is on various features of discourse it does not start with linguistic categories (sentences, sub-clauses, etc.) to explicate that discourse.  Such categories may or may not be relevant to what is going on but as they are derived from an abstract analysis of linguistic structure it is quite possible that they will not be.

Second, these are not volumes on the psychology of language or psycholinguistics.  There is a large body of work that is concerned with a wide range of issues to do with language learning, sentence production, speech perception and so on (see e.g. Hartley, 2001).  This is a very different enterprise from that of the work in the current collection.  One fundamental difference is that psycholinguistics works with a cognitivist and information processing view of the human actor and attempts to explicate the cognitive machinery that is taken to underlie and generate talk.  The studies collected here avoid this kind of technical decomposition of psychology into a set of modules and processes; instead it starts with the practical situation of the actor embedded in interaction.  Issues such as what the speaker intends, hears or mishears, what is confused or clear are therefore treated  as practical and local issues to be worked out interactionally. 

Third, these are not volumes on the social psychology of language.  Although discursive psychology as is exemplified by the papers in Volume 3 has been treated as a perspective within the social psychology of language (see Robinson & Giles, 2001) much of the social psychology of language is concerned with topics addressed in a more traditionally cognitivist manner. 

Fourth, these are not volumes on discourse processes.  Much of the discourse processes work grew up through the cross fertilization of work in pragmatics and speech act theory with more traditional psycholinguistics.  There are a range of different approaches here.  Mostly, as the name implies, they are concerned with cognitive processes associated with discourse – story comprehension, the perception of discourse cohesion and so on.  Discourse processes is a less clearly defined area than psycholinguistics (see contributions to van Dijk, 1996) and some of the contributions this volume could be seen as part of discourse processes or the more general discourse studies tradition.  But its centre of gravity is outside the pull of this volume.

Sixth, these volumes do not collect together research in critical discourse analysis (CDA).  Again, the contrast is not so clear cut, as CDA covers a broad set of approaches (see Fairclough and Wodak, 1997, for a review) and there is a strong critical strand in the work collected in the current volumes, particularly in the contributions to social psychological debates about racism, nationalism and ideology in Volume 2.  Indeed, in their influential overview Phillips and Jørgensen (2002) suggest that discursive psychology (the main theme of Volume 3) satisfies many of the defining features of CDA but does not use the title.  Nevertheless, CDA work tends to start with linguistic analysis, often the functional grammar associated with Michael Halliday (Halliday, 1985).  Often CDA work attempts to connect textual structures to social structures rather than focus more directly at the activities done through talk as is characteristic of much of the work discussed here.  Moreover, this leads to differences in the way ‘discourse’ itself is conceptualized. 

As I have noted, in the work collected in this volume discourse is treated in terms of language in use, a focus on talk and texts being parts of practices.  In CDA it is more typical to treat discourses as objects of some kind that can be counted.  This is often inspired by Foucaultian thinking, although there is considerable debate as to the precise conceptualization of discourse in Foucault’s work (see Hepburn, 2003).  The Potter et al. Chapter 13 of the current volume explores the different conceptions of discourse and offers some reasons for caution about CDA constructions such as ‘the discourse of medicine’ or ‘the discourse of science’.  The work in these volumes is more likely to see such discourse organizations as a by-product of the way activities are done within settings rather than a consequence of the internal logic of discourse structures. 

Finally, these are not volumes of conversation analysis (CA).   There is a degree of overlap between the work collected here and work in CA.  However, most CA is not focused on psychological issues.  It is only recently that there has been a sustained engagement with questions of cognition and interaction in CA, and there is by no means an agreed approach to the topic (see te Molder & Potter, 2005 for examples).  Nevertheless, CA exerts a strong influence on the studies here, particularly in Volume 3, and there are a range of contributions by researchers who would probably describe themselves as conversation analysts.  There is a full collection of CA work to parallel this one (Drew & Heritage, 2006); I have chosen here a selection of pieces that have been particularly influential or have highlighted, often implicitly, issues about the relation of discourse and psychology.  Contributions by Watson, Drew, Pomerantz, Suchman & Jordan and Houtkoop-Steenstra & Antaki in this volume, combine with the contribution of Wooffitt to Volume 2 and the pieces by Goodwin and Wooffitt in Volume 3 illustrate the important contribution of CA to this tradition of work.

The selection of papers for these volumes is inevitably a personal choice (although I canvassed a range of opinions of others).  The aim has been to give a selection of the best and most influential work.  I checked citations and where there are a range of choices to capture a particular topic of strand of argument I typically chose the paper that citations suggested had been most influential.  A particularly important criterion has also been clarity – the selections are designed to offer a narrative that covers the different strands and the development of this tradition of work.  With Volume 1 the choices are most open ended as for this early work there is no easy start point or canon to work with.  Often there are a range of roughly equivalent possibilities and I have attempted to choose pieces that are most accessible to a wider audience.  Inevitably not everyone would agree with these choices.  And there are plenty of high quality and influential pieces that lost out purely for space considerations.  For example, I thought about reproducing Mills’ (1940) seminal piece on vocabularies of motive, although this would probably need to be complemented by something illustrating Wittgenstein’s take on psychological language, and Austin’s (1961) work on accounts and excuses, and then further pieces locating some of the post-structural strands in early discourse work.  This would inevitably have diluted the collection.  Some readers might be surprised by the omission of work from the short lived journal Ideology & Consciousness or something of the Foucault inspired work represented in Henriques et al. (1984).  However, I have judged that it is hard to do justice that that strand of work in a brief selection and it is anyway somewhat marginal to the tradition documented here.  Again, I have opted for focus and clarity.

 

Themes in Discourse and Psychology

 

There is no neat history of the earliest phase of discourse and psychology work.  Indeed, work in the sociology of scientific knowledge (see Chapter 12 by Mulkay & Gilbert) tells us that reconstructions of research fields this kind are always partial and work to simplify events and reinterpret them in terms of contemporary categories.  Academic history making is bound by a range of textual conventions and pushes a range of different agendas as Stringer shows in Chapter 3.  I have chosen to break things up into a series of themes but readers should hold in mind that these are post hoc reconstructions designed to make intellectual contributions clearer in contemporary terms.

 

Linguistic philosophy and practices of mind

 

One of the central figures that inspired the rethinking of psychology illustrated here was the philosopher Ludwig Wittgenstein.  Wittgenstein’s work was notoriously difficult and fragmented and there has been much debate over its implications.  Coulter’s contribution in Chapter 1 offers a reading of Wittgensteinian philosophy that brings out its implications for academic psychology in its various cognitive forms.  He takes the terms ‘understanding’ and ‘intending’ as his central examples and illustrates problems with interpretations of these words that treat them as referring to private events within the person.  In contrast, he develops the practical interpretation that has these words as being counters to be drawn on in different kinds of language games which people engage in.  For example, ‘understanding’ is often used to mark a claim to success.  However, such a claim is not warrantable by reference to an inner event or experience (whatever such a thing would be) but is dependent on practical, public and social criteria (maybe a person tries out the ‘understanding’ and finds it flawed, or other people my doubt its veracity – the criteria for ‘understanding’ are social rather than individual). 

Wittgenstein develops a picture of all language, including the language of psychology, as a toolbox:

Think of the tools in a tool-box: there is a hammer, pliers, a saw, a screw-driver, a rule, a glue-pot, glue, nails and screws.—The functions of words are as diverse as the functions of these objects…  Of course, what confuses us is the uniform appearance of words when we hear them spoken or meet them in script and print.  For their application is not presented to us so clearly.  (Wittgenstein, 1953: #11)

One of the ways of understanding the development of the tradition of discourse research laid out in these three volumes is as a programme of making that application more present so it can be more clearly understood.  For Wittgenstein (and Coulter, 1983) this largely involves conceptual analysis – for the work here it increasingly involves the audio recorder and transcript.  We can see Wittgensteinian themes developed in different ways, with a particular focus on the self and its coherence, in Harré’s Chapter 4 and Shotter’s Chapter 6.  The public and conventional nature of language use emphasised by Wittgenstein is a key feature of a wide range of discourse work including studies in ethnomethodology, rhetoric, conversation analysis and discursive psychology.

 

Ethnomethodology and the production of factuality

 

A further central problematic in work at the boundary of discourse and psychology brings together issues of epistemics, constructionism, description and psychology.  Smith’s study in Chapter 2 was influential in showing how factual descriptions and they way they operate can be studied.  It picks up from Garfinkel’s ethnomethodology which highlights the everyday methods that people use to produce descriptions of the social world that are treated as rational, appropriate and justifiable (Garfinkel, 1967).  Smith’s analysis is doubly interesting from a psychological perspective.  On the one hand, it foregrounds the various practical, discursive methods that speakers use to make versions seem literal and objective.  On the other, it shows how constructions of persons can be put together to be parts of particular actions, in this extreme case to produce a critical account of a person as mentally ill.  Smith’s analysis focused on particular words and constructions at a level of specificity that was hitherto extremely unusual in analytic work. 

Smith took seriously Harvey Sacks’ proposal – a proposal central to the field of conversation analysis – that talk is organized in its specifics and that any order of detail is potentially related to interaction (see Sacks, 1992).  We will see this followed through increasingly in research studies as we progress through the 3 volumes.  Although Smith did not characterise her work in this way, we can see in it a form of discourse constructionism rather different from the social constructionism that became commonplace in later social science.  Constructionism here involves particular discourse constructions being organized in to establish a particular version as part of a particular action.  This focus on the organization of description complements the approach taken by Mulkay & Gilbert in Chapter 12.

 

Post-structuralism, reflexivity and the sociology of scientific knowledge

 

One of the features of 1970s and 1980s social science was the engagement with ideas from post-structuralist thinkers such as Barthes, Derrida and Foucault, either directly or indirectly through work such as Henriques et al. (1984).  Research in this tradition developed a very different notion of discourse that emphasised its systemic and structural properties and connected issues of discourse and ideology (for a review of the implications of post-structuralism for discourse work more generally see Hepburn 2003). 

This engagement is represented here by Stringer’s Chapter 3.  This draws on and explores various post-structural problematics.  It merges these with concerns about sociology of scientific knowledge as it considers the various representations of a psychological research field that are variously available in textbooks, research papers and informal conversation.  Picking up from Derrida’s work that explores issues of representation and figuration in language partly through pushing the boundaries of language use to destruction, Stringer displays his argument partly through the form of his article.  This reflexive argument, using an alternative literary form was an important feature of the early development of this work – see for example Ashmore (1989) or for a more psychological case Curt (1994).  Such formal experiments have generally remained part of the meta-theorizing in this area rather than influencing how analytic studies were produced.

 

  The crisis in social psychology

 

An important impetus to the development of an interest in discourse work and its relation to psychology comes from the upheavals in social psychology associated with the so called ‘crisis in social psychology’ in the 1970s.  Although in the longer term this had relatively little impact in US social psychology, elsewhere it was a stimulus to major transformation.  The crisis had three broad strands.  First was a critique of individualism that reacted against social psychology’s increasing focus on individual cognitive explanations.  Part of this critique was political, but there were also major theoretical and analytic reasons for doubting psychology’s individualism.  The second strand of the crisis was a critique of method.  This was centred on a feeling of dissatisfaction with the narrowness of experimental work and its limitations for understanding human action.  Third came a critique of theory, and particularly the failure of social psychological theory to address issues of social organization and social structure.  The work in discourse and psychology in the current volumes offers a general response to this crisis with its reformulation of theory and method and its move away from individual cognitive explanations.

Key figures in this crisis were Gergen, Harré and Shotter who are all represented in this volume.  The pieces reproduced here are indicative of a broader strand of work and highlight particular discursive themes.  Gergen has gone on to be a major figure in a broader constructionist perspective (e.g. Gergen 1994).  Harré is particularly associated with the analytic programme of ethogenics and strongly Wittgenstein influenced analytic work (see Harré & Gillett, 1994).  Shotter too has gone on to focus particularly on the implications of Wittgenstein for psychology(e.g. Shotter, 1993).  He has also been important for engaging with the Russian literary theorist Bakhtin who has been a key figure in the development of activity theory (see Wertsch, 1991).     

 

Conversation analysis and a discursive conception of psychology

 

Away from the debates within the discipline of psychology, developments in ethnomethodology and conversation analysis were offering new ways of conceptualizing psychology almost as a by-product of their main interest.  The chapters in this volume by Bogen & Lynch, Watson, Drew and Pomerantz do not thematize the issue of psychology.  Nevertheless, they start to show the potential for this kind of interaction research offering a very different picture of psychology.  Bogen & Lynch focused on the role of constructions of memory appear in official testimony, particularly as speakers manage the plausible deniability of their positions.  For example, they explore the way Oliver North’s practices of claiming not to remember in his testimony to the Iran-Contra investigation committee provide for avoiding troubling lines of questioning.  A key feature of this approach is that instead of treating memory as an inner cognitive object to be investigated using the methods of psychology it is treated as a practical move in an interactional practice of managing guilt and innocence within a particular institution.  This line of thinking is extended in chapters by Goodwin and Edwards in volume 3.

Watson and Drew both work with legal materials and are focused on issues of description that have since become core in the sorts of discourse analysis and discursive psychology collected in volumes 2 and 3.  Watson draws on Sacks’ membership categorization analysis to show the way murder suspects provide descriptions of their victims that invoke various categories that manage issues of culpability and mitigation.  In contrast to the work on categories and categorization in social psychology, which focuses on the role of categories in relation to information processing, here categories are studied as words and categorization as socially located practices of using those words to perform particular actions.  Drew too studied the working of descriptive constructions, but this time his focus was on the way descriptions work in the cross examination where a witness to an alleged rape deals with cross examination.  Drew highlights the role of contrasting descriptions from the Counsel and the witness in constructing different attitudes, motives and moral status of the parties involved.  Fundamental psychological matters are played out through these practices of description.

Pomerantz drew more directly on conversation analysis in her study of the practice of indirectly ‘fishing’ for information from an interactant.  Fishing involves a party telling something that they only have partial knowledge of to someone who has, or is entitled to have, full knowledge of that thing.  For example, ‘your line’s been busy’ is a description from the point of view of caller, but the recipient has been on the phone and is expected to know who they were talking to.  Eliciting the information about who the recipient was talking to by fishing in this way is less invasive than a direct question.  Pomerantz’s original focus was purely on the nature of this interactional device, but we can read this study as a contribution to discursive psychology.  It shows how particular uses of description, invocations of knowledge and experience and the person’s position related to those things, is drawn on for practical tasks.  Epistemology is a live interactional issue here in a way that was startlingly different from the psychology of knowledge and belief. 

 

Rhetoric and the disruption of cognitivism

 

Billig’s contribution to the area of discourse and psychology comes from neither the ethnomethodological and conversation analytic tradition, nor the post-structural tradition; instead he drew on classical rhetoric to engage with and rework cognitivist thinking in psychology.  Billig argued that attitudes and assertions should be understood in their rhetorical context.  Thus an attitude makes sense in the context of potential alternatives where it can be understood as a potential argumentative counter to those alternatives.  Such an approach moves the study of attitudes from an issue of individual cognition to an issue of public assertions and their argumentative counters (see for example Billig’s Chapter 28). 

The piece reproduced in Chapter 11 is one of the first articles to set out this perspective as well as one of the first pieces of analysis of this kind published in a traditional social psychology journal.  In it Billig develops a critique of social identity theory with a classical rhetorical inversion – he shows the way that the processes of categorization that had been the main topic of social identity theory must be understood in relation to equally important processes of particularization when they are addressed in the context of specific arguments.  Shotter’s Chapter 6 offers an account of how Billig’s rhetorical perspective can be linked in to both Bakhtinian thinking and the linguistic philosophy of Wittgenstein.  In general, this strand of rhetorical work provides another element in the move from an analysis of mental processes to an analysis of practical actions.

 

Facts, accounts and interpretative repertoires

 

A key strand in the early work on discourse and psychology came out of the sociology of scientific knowledge.  This was a research field that developed careful empirical studies of the operation of scientific fields with the aim of understanding processes in the development of knowledge, theory change and so on (for a relevant review of the considerable and sometimes controversial theoretical complexity in this field see Potter, 1996, ch. 1).  The contribution of Mulkay and Gilbert in Chapter 12 emphasised the centrality of scientific discourse and in particular the organization of accounts for what is taken to be true and false.  They introduced the idea of an interpretative repertoire to help understand the operation of discourse, and in particular to make sense of the huge variation that they found in the accounts of the scientists they were studying when they looked at them closely.  In effect, this is a semi-organized, semi-preformed resource for action – it can be used for justifying, criticising, and producing versions of the world which appear solid and objective.  Contrasting versions of actions and events are constructed out of constructing repertoires.  

This analytic notion has become widely used – it is drawn on in many of the chapters in Volume 2.  It is interesting to note that when it was introduced by Mulkay and Gilbert it was related to the notion of linguistic register brought to prominence by the linguist Halliday (1978).  However, the specific way repertoires were defined, and the subsequent analytic use of the notion quickly departed from the notion of register.  The concept of an interpretative repertoire is developed further in Chapter 13 by Potter et al. who lay out some of the benefits of the notion and compare it to some versions of the Foucaultian notion of discourse.  Debates about the relative merits of repertoires, discourses and other possible notions such as social representations have continued.  It is notable that although the notion of repertoires is a central feature of many of the social psychological contributions seen in Volume 2 it is largely absent from the discursive psychology collected together in Volume 3.

 

Methods in Practice and Methods as Topic

 

One of the features of this tradition of work that separates it out from other social psychological perspectives that emerged from the crisis in social psychology, for example Harré’s ethogenics perspective (1979) or the work on social representations associated with Moscovici (1984), is its focus on method.  This is partly because of the early interest in the limitations of social psychological methods such as experiments and surveys and partly because of the broader interest in the way methods operate in science. 

Following through the emphasis on language in use had important implications for the way method is understood.  Initially the idea that people’s descriptions were parts of actions raised problems for any method that treated people’s talk as merely descriptive of what they thought, of what had happened to them, of their social environment and so on.  The initial move was to use open ended interviews as a procedure not for harvesting people’s views or information about their past or whatever, but as an arena for revealing the different interpretative repertoires they used to construct the world as part of specific actions.  The Potter & Mulkay Chapter 14 is the earliest full statement of this perspective on interviews, and this stance on interviews has informed large amounts of discourse analytic work done since.

As discourse research became more refined and subtle, and as it drew increasingly on conversation analytic understandings of the activities done in talk, the earlier perspective on interviews seemed more limited.  Indeed, interviews started to seem more complex and harder to analyse than had previously been thought.  Potter & Hepburn’s Chapter 16 is an exploration of some of the varied considerations that must be addressed when conducting, analysing and presenting interviews.  Antaki et al’s Chapter 17 highlights some of the analytic shortcomings that have become widespread in the large body of interview work done in the last two decades.  Indeed, it highlights just the considerations that have encouraged researchers to increasingly move toward the study of naturalistic records – audio and video recordings of people interacting in their institutional and everyday settings.  This move will become clear from the pattern of contributions; while many of the contributions to Volume 2 depend on interview material most of the contributions to Volume 3 work with naturalistic materials.  Edwards Chapter 15 is an illustration of what is possible in applying the insights from contemporary interaction analysis and discursive psychology to interview talk.  Although the study highlights further limitations in the use of interviews, it also reveals new analytic possibilities. 

The final theme in this section of articles on method is what is revealed by an application of interaction research and discursive psychology to the study of methods in action.  The Suchman and Jordan Chapter 18 was one of the first pieces that took method as topic in this way.  Their study of standardized surveys highlighted the failure of survey researchers to conceptualize interaction adequately, and showed up how this failure undermined the goal of standardization.  It made the challenging claim that to achieve standardization for the recipients of survey questions interviewers might need to respond flexibly to the contingencies of natural conversation.  Myers in Chapter 19 and Houtkoop-Steenstra & Antaki in Chapter 20 are both studies that consider the way the classic objects of psychological research (‘opinions’, ‘quality of life’) are at least partly produced through interactions in the research setting.  These latter papers raise profound questions for psychological research methods and their status vis a vis the objects of psychological study. 

 

References

 

Ashmore, M. (1989). The reflexive thesis: Wrighting sociology of scientific knowledge. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Austin, J. (1961). A plea for excuses. In J.D. Urmson & G. Warnock (Eds.), Philosophical papers. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Coulter, J. (1983). Contingent and a priori structures in sequential analysis, Human Studies, 6, 361-76.

Curt, B.C. (1994). Textuality and tectonics: Troubling social and psychological science. Buckingham, UK and Philadelphia, PA: Open University Press.

Drew, P.& Heritage, J. (Eds)(2006).  Conversation analysis, vol. 1.  London: Sage.

Garfinkel, H. (1967). Studies in ethnomethodology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Gergen, K. J. (1994). Realities and relationships. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Halliday, M.A.K. (1978). Language as Social Semiotic: the social interpretation of language and meaning. London: Edward Arnold.

Halliday, M.A.K. (1985).  An introduction to functional grammar. London: Edward Arnold.

Harré, R. (1979). Social being: A theory for social psychology.  Oxford: Blackwell.

Harré, R. & Gillett, G. (1994).  The discursive mind.  London: Sage.

Hartley, T.A. (2001).  The Psychology of Language: From data to theory, 2nd Ed.  Psychology Press.

Henriques, J., Hollway, W., Urwin, C., Venn, C. and Walkerdine, V. (1984). Changing the subject: Psychology, social regulation and subjectivity. London: Methuen.

Hepburn, A. (2003). An Introduction to Critical Social Psychology. London: Sage.

Jay, T.B. (2003).  The psychology of language.  Prentice Hall.

Mills, C.W. (1940). Situated actions and vocabularies of motive, American Sociological Review, 5, 904-13. 

Moscovici, S. (1984). The phenomenon of social representations.  In R.M. Farr & S. Moscovici (Eds). Social representations.  Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Phillips, L.J. & Jørgenson, M.W. (2002). Discourse analysis as theory and method.  London; Sage.

Sacks, H. (1992). Lectures on conversation. Vols. I & II, edited by G. Jefferson. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

Shotter, J. (1993). Conversational realities: Constructing life through language. London: Sage.

te Molder, H. & Potter, J. (Eds) (2005). Conversation and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

van Dijk, T.A. (1996) Discourse as structure and process (vol 1 of Discourse studies: A multidisciplinary introduction).  London; Sage.

Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the mind: A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester.